Pluralistic ignorance—a shared
misperception of how others think or behave—poses a challenge to
collective action on problems like climate change. Using a
representative sample of Americans (N = 6119), we examine
whether Americans accurately perceive national concern about
climate change and support for mitigating policies. We find a form
of pluralistic ignorance that we describe as a false social
reality: a near universal perception of public opinion that is
the opposite of true public sentiment. Specifically, 80–90% of
Americans underestimate the prevalence of support for major
climate change mitigation policies and climate concern. While
66–80% Americans support these policies, Americans estimate the
prevalence to only be between 37–43% on average. Thus, supporters
of climate policies outnumber opponents two to one, while
Americans falsely perceive nearly the opposite to be true.
Further, Americans in every state and every assessed demographic
underestimate support across all polices tested. Preliminary
evidence suggests three sources of these misperceptions: (i)
consistent with a false consensus effect, respondents who support
these policies less (conservatives) underestimate support by a
greater degree; controlling for one’s own personal politics, (ii)
exposure to more conservative local norms and (iii) consuming
conservative news correspond to greater misperceptions.
Introduction
Addressing a collective action problem
like climate change requires individuals to recognize the problem
as a threat and to engage in coordinated actions that result in
major structural and social change. Collective action problems
pose a difficult challenge as individuals are less likely to act
when there are others who standby and do nothing—and this outcome
is only more common when the problem at hand is not clearly
perceived to be a threat1.
Further, research on threat perception suggests we rely heavily on
others’ reactions to recognize complex or non-immediate problems
like climate change as a threat2,3.
Similarly, perceptions of social norms, including perceiving
others’ concern and desire for action, may be key to coordinating
collective solutions4,5,
including in environmental domains6,7.
But such processes are hampered if people fail to accurately
perceive that others are concerned and support taking action.
Systematic misperception of public opinion (i.e., “pluralistic
ignorance”8)
like a widespread underestimation of public support for climate
action could inhibit willingness to talk about the problem with
others8,9,
and could lead people to falsely conclude that the vocal minority
who dismiss climate change are representative of broader public
opinion10.
Further, given that most Americans report concern about climate
change and support many policies to address the issue11,12,
why has the US not yet enacted major climate policy to address the
issue? If most Americans were unaware of the popularity of their
pro-climate action views, this could encourage inaction through
pressures to conform to the (mis)perceived political attitudes of
others, a phenomenon robust across the political spectrum13.
These concerning possibilities raise the question: Do Americans
accurately perceive public support for climate mitigation?
Generally, our perceptions about the
world, including the social world, are shaped by society and can
be thought of as part of a “social reality”, wherein some social
truths are widely held and can exert influence over us14,15.
Notably, whether or not these perceptions are accurate, they can
shape our actions and beliefs, including our expectations or
judgment of others16.
To better understand the impact social realities, the role of
second order beliefs (our beliefs about others’ beliefs) are
increasingly highlighted as important contributors to and
intervention point for contemporary social problems17,18.
Indeed, there have been calls for a better understanding of the
social determinants of collective behavior to be elevated to a
major “crisis discipline” of our time19.
Here, we investigate norm misperception in the climate policy
context. Pluralistic ignorance refers to a systematic and shared
misperception of a norm, where many people have the same
misconception about what most people do or think20.
For instance, college students have been found to collectively
misperceive that drinking is more common and desirable among their
fellow students than is actually the case21.
In addition to perceptions of local community norms, pluralistic
ignorance can also pertain to society-wide misperceptions17.
For example, people may succumb to the “conservative bias”,
whereby perceptions of public opinion lag behind actual public
opinion by some decades, failing to reflect changes and anchoring
on historic levels22.
The conservative bias is particularly likely when public opinion
has recently changed on a topic but policy and structural change
has not yet resulted from this shift, leaving little concrete
indication of a shift in norms22.
Previous work suggests there may be
pluralistic ignorance on climate concern and some related policies
amongst the US public. First, research on a related topic suggests
that people systematically overestimate the percentage of others
in their country who outright reject the existence of human-caused
climate change, with representative samples from US, China, and
Australia showing that although most in each country believe in
man-made climate change, people underestimate the extent to which
their fellow citizens do23,24,25.
And, research using student samples has found that college
students underestimate their peers concern about climate change,
broadly8.
More pertinent to climate policy, one study found that an online
convenience sample underestimated Americans’ support to regulate
CO2 as a pollutant (broadly, not in a specific policy framework),
and concern about climate change26.
Similarly, research on a sample of U.S. congressional staffers
found that many underestimated the popularity of carbon pollution
restrictions among the public in their district27.
Further, work in the northeastern U.S. coastal states using an
online convenience sample of Americans found that most
underestimate support for regional decarbonization approaches like
expanding offshore wind28.
While piecemeal and unrepresentative, these scattered indicators
are a cause for concern—one that demands a conclusive
investigation of climate policy pluralistic ignorance using a
representative sample to investigate concrete, major national
climate policies.
Given the possible role of
pluralistic ignorance in stalling progress on this existential
threat, it is prudent to investigate fundamental, unanswered
questions about pluralistic ignorance on climate policy support:
Is pluralistic ignorance around climate policy common in the U.S.?
Is it contained to specific pockets of Americans, or does it span
many populations over many geographies? Does it affect only
specific policies, or does it hold for a variety of climate change
mitigation policies (e.g., those utilizing market instruments, as
opposed to mandates, or direct investment in infrastructure)? As
prior research has found that Americans underestimate fellow
Americans’ belief in climate change23,
it’s possible that they also underestimate public support across a
range of climate change mitigation policies (i.e., they may expect
lower policy support for any policy addressing a problem if they
assume others don’t believe that the problem exists). If
pluralistic ignorance is present in this context, how large are
the misperceptions? Do some misperceptions exist, but perceptions
are accurate about majority and minority opinion? Or do they
surpass this level and result in misperceiving what the majority
of Americans support?
The main objective of the present
study is to provide clear and granular answers to the above
questions. Additionally, there may be questions about the possible
sources of misperception. One contributor to misperceiving popular
opinion could be false consensus, where people pay selective
attention to others’ beliefs that are similar to their own, and
overestimate the number of people who agree with them29.
Indeed, false consensus effects exist for belief in climate change
and support for local renewable energy projects in the Northeast
US24,28.
As such, we might expect that U.S. conservatives underestimate
support for climate mitigation policies to a greater extent as
they have lower approval of said policies.
Second, when forming estimates of
frequency or probability, people’s guesses are generally shaped by
information that is more easily available or retrievable,
reflecting an availability heuristic30.
Thus, people’s estimates of national public opinion may show an
outsized influence of local norms that are easier to witness
firsthand or recall. Given this, people’s estimates of the nation
as a whole may be swayed by their state-level norms such that
those in more conservative states and those in states with fewer
climate protests may underestimate climate policy support to a
greater degree.
A third possible contributor is media
consumption, particularly if media misrepresent public opinion31.
News media coverage of scientific experts in the U.S. has
historically given disproportionately too much time to climate
change deniers32
and presentations of conservatives as oppositional to climate
change policy, while the conservative electorate is actually
fairly divided on these issues12.
Given differences in media coverage, one might expect that public
opinion misperceptions would be particularly pronounced amongst
those who consume news outlets that have been shown to be more
inaccuracy-prone33.
In the present work, we investigate
national misperceptions of support for transformative climate
policies and broader concern about climate change and show that
Americans experience pluralistic ignorance to such a magnitude and
breadth that it can be considered a false social reality:
Americans from all walks of life systematically underestimate
public concern about climate change and policy support over a
range of climate policies. The magnitude of the effect is such
that those who want action are a supermajority (i.e., 66% or
higher), while there is a ubiquitous perception across
demographics that they are only a minority.
Results
Study overview
To create a detailed picture of the state
of pluralistic ignorance for climate policy, we use a large stratified
sample of US adults (N = 6,119) through the Ipsos eNation
Omnibus nationally representative panel to compare public opinion on
climate change to perceptions of popularity of those same opinions. We
commissioned this panel to oversample less-populous states to assess
the extent of pluralistic ignorance for each state with greater
precision, and aiming for a 10% margin of error for all states. For
the full sample, this sample size is more than 80% powered to detect
small national levels of pluralistic ignorance (effects as small as
d = 0.04), as well as being 80% powered to detect separate levels
among Democrat, Republican, and Independent partisan groups (effects
as small as d = 0.07), allowing for very granular comparisons.
For all national-level analyses, we applied weights from the survey
provider to ensure representativeness (e.g., down-weighting data from
smaller states that we oversampled).
Actual levels of U.S. public support on
climate policies were obtained from nationally representative public
opinion data available from Yale’s Program on Climate Change
Communication (YPCCC), who used the same survey provider who collected
the data for the present study (which may help account for any
surveyor-specific sampling or data practices)11,12.
We also use YPCCC’s state level estimates of public opinion (which are
estimated from their national survey data via multilevel regression
with post-stratification). These data were collected during the same
year as the data in the present study (see Methods). Further, to avoid
any differences in policy support estimates due to item wording in
comparing actual levels to responses from our panel, we used precisely
the same policy descriptions as used by YPCCC. Thus, if wording for
any of the policy items is subtly leading to inflated (or deflated)
support numbers, this wording should also lead to inflated (or
deflated) estimates of opinion estimates; thus, specific item wording
would be unlikely to create discrepancies between actual and perceived
policy support.
We asked participants to estimate the
percent of Americans who were at least somewhat concerned about
climate change (see Methods for full survey text, and a discussion
about using the phrase “climate change” vs “global warming” for this
item). We then chose a set of specific climate policies especially
relevant to the decarbonization of the US and the attainment of
climate mitigation goals such as the 2015 Paris Agreement. We
intentionally selected a set of climate change mitigation policies
that varied in core features such as utilizing market instruments as
opposed to mandates, or those that facilitate investment and the
creation of infrastructure. For each policy, we asked participants to
estimate the percent of Americans who would support it. Our list of
policies included support for a carbon tax levied against fossil fuel
companies and redistributed to Americans through tax breaks. The list
also included a renewable energy standard that mandates 100%
electricity generated by renewable energy in the near term—an
essential step in decarbonizing our energy production34.
And, as decarbonizing our energy infrastructure will require rapidly
siting of wind and solar across the US, we also included support for
siting renewables on public lands34.
Given the need to consider infrastructure, jobs, and social equity in
transitioning to renewable energy, we also asked participants to
estimate the support for the Green New Deal (GND). Notably, large
environmental policy packages like the GND and the American Jobs Plan
may play a key role in passing environmental legislation, as research
shows that bundling more redistributive, social equity, and
job-creating measures into major environmental policies makes them
more popular35.
Finally, we asked participants about
their news consumption, political affiliation, and demographic
characteristics, which we used to conduct an exploratory
cross-sectional analysis of possible sources of pluralistic ignorance
in norm perceptions.
Prevalence and magnitude of
pluralistic ignorance
Figure 1
shows that the vast majority of Americans greatly underestimate how
many of their fellow Americans worry about climate change and support
transformative climate policies to remedy the situation. While most
Americans believe that less than half of the country is worried about
climate change (Mest = 43.3), in actuality it is
two-thirds (Mreal = 66), t(6118) = 70.9, d = 0.92,
P < 0.001, 95% CIdiff = [22.0, 23.3] (see Methods
for additional notes on these analyses). Americans’ estimates for
major climate policy support is the same or even lower (Ms = 37–43%),
when in fact two-thirds of the country or more support each of these
policies (Table 1).
The distributions of these estimates in Fig. 1
show two noticeable concentrations, one at around 50% and another
around 25%, salient proportions that seem to serve as focal points for
answering these questions, even though a similarly salient
proportion—75%—would have been a far more accurate answer. The
misperceptions in estimates are so robust that, for every item
assessed, the estimates of the lowest 25% and of the middle 50% of
respondents falls well below the true values. More precisely, between
79% and 88% of our national sample underestimate public concern or
each policy support.
Boxes inside the violin plot represent the middle 50% of the sample,
with a line at the median, while the minima and maxima illustrated
represent the full range of responses (from 0–100%). The red line
represents the true prevalence, as indicated by contemporaneous
national polling12,43.
RE refers to renewable energy. N = 6119 survey participants.
We also asked participants for estimates
of support in their home state, and found these perceptions (when
averaged across states) to not vary substantially from the
national-level estimates and to have very similar distributions (see
Supplementary Fig. 1
in the Supplementary Information File). Overall, this pattern of
results suggests that people misperceive support for climate action
broadly, having non-specific and robust misestimates for support for a
variety of climate policies. Indeed, in an exploratory factor analysis
of the five responses shown in Fig. 1,
a single factor emerged (all other factors had eigenvalues <1).
Pluralistic ignorance across
partisans and policies
Breaking these perceptions of national
public concern and policy support down by partisan politics, we found
that Democrats, Independents, and Republicans all estimate levels for
climate concern and climate policy support below 50%, while actual
values are much higher. However, Fig. 2
shows that Republicans’ estimates were reliably lower than Democrats’
by 5–12%, with Independents falling somewhere in between.
All judgments were elicited for everyone in
either the entire nation or one’s state (not only for one’s fellow
partisans). Items on the left labeled with “US” indicate estimates of
support for policies at the national level, while those on the right
labeled with “State” indicate estimates of support for those in
participants’ own state. Actual partisan support levels available from
contemporaneous polling (available for four US policies12)
are indicated by squares. Error bars represent 95% confidence
intervals for the norm perception means illustrated. RE refers to
renewable energy. GND refers to the Green New Deal. N = 6119
survey participants, including 2777 Democrats, 1022 Independents, and
2320 Republicans.
For the national policy items,
contemporaneous polling was available broken down by partisans, so we
can compare partisans’ estimates of nationwide support to actual
partisan levels of support. Figure 2
shows that when Democrats, Independents, and Republicans estimate how
the nation feels on these issues, their estimates of other Americans’
support for these policies only really resemble actual Republican
levels of policy support. In fact, even if individuals’ estimates for
the nation as a whole were, for some reason, based solely on
Republican levels of support, all partisan groups would still be
underestimating support for policies like a carbon tax and siting
renewables on public lands. While differences between partisans are
consistent with false consensus effects (e.g., Democrats—who are more
likely to personally support climate policy—tend to provide relatively
higher estimates of others’ policy support than do Republicans), these
effects are dwarfed by the absolute levels of misperception held by
all Americans that strongly underestimates climate policy support.
For all policies and concern for climate
change at the national level, as well as state level support for a
carbon tax and concern for climate change, we were able to compare
each participant’s norm estimates to available contemporaneous data on
actual support levels. Doing so, we create difference scores for each
participant’s estimate, which can be aggregated into pluralistic
ignorance levels that we analyze below.
Reflecting the lower norm estimates by
Republicans, Fig. 3
shows that Republicans’ opinion misperceptions are stronger in
magnitude than Democrats’ and Independents’ across all items. Further,
we find that all partisan groups underestimate concern for climate
change at both the national and state level by roughly 20–30%. In
policy support, we find that the magnitude of misperception is highest
for support to site renewables on public lands, with underestimates
closer to 35–40%. Underestimation is smaller for support for
100-percent renewable energy mandates, which is still between 20–25%
lower than actual levels. Support for a carbon tax and a Green New
Deal fall in between these levels.
Pluralistic ignorance is calculated as the
difference between real and perceived norm levels, so greater values
indicate real levels are higher than perceived norms (i.e. greater
underestimation). All perceptions pertain to either the entire nation
or one’s state (not one’s fellow partisans). Items on the left labeled
with “US” indicate estimates of support for policies at the national
level, while those on the right labeled with “State” indicate
estimates of support for those in participants’ own state. Error bars
represent 95% confidence intervals of the mean differences
illustrated. RE refers to renewable energy, and GND refers to the
Green New Deal. N = 6119 survey participants, including 2777
Democrats, 1022 Independents, and 2320 Republicans.
We can also directly compare state and
national pluralistic ignorance levels for the two items for which we
have data for both (a carbon tax and worry about climate change) to
test if estimates are more accurate for state than national items.
Using a mixed model to predict pluralistic ignorance levels across
these four items using a dummy-coded fixed effect for item location
(state = 0 vs nation = 1) and random intercepts for participant and
item type (carbon tax vs worry), we find no difference between the
two, t(21762) = −0.94, P = 0.350, suggesting that people are
equally inaccurate at estimating opinions of fellow denizens of their
own state, relative to the entire US public.
Regional variation
Aggregating levels of pluralistic
ignorance by state, we can map the magnitude of false norm perceptions
across the country. Figure 4
shows that residents of all states underestimate how much the
nation is worried about climate change and support climate policy (for
separate maps for each policy, see Supplemental Fig. 2
and Supplemental Fig. 3).
For both perceived popular worry and climate policy support, we see
that the southern gulf states (e.g., Mississippi) tend to show the
highest pluralistic ignorance. But, underscoring the ubiquity of this
misperception, even liberal states such as California and New York
underestimate climate policy support as much as many conservative
states. In fact, no state was less than 20% off in their estimates of
climate policy support. These errors are robust for the more proximal
state-level estimates as well, where participants of virtually every
state underestimated how concerned their fellow state residents were
about climate change, and how much they supported a carbon tax (see
Supplemental Fig. 4).
A shows pluralistic ignorance levels
for worry about climate change by taking the difference from real and
perceived national levels of climate change and averaging those levels
across participants in each state. B shows levels of
pluralistic ignorance for climate policy, calculated by averaging the
difference between real and perceived support levels across the four
national policies examined for each participant, and then averaging
those levels of national policy support misperception for all
participants in each state. N = 6119 survey participants. In
both panels, greater values indicate real levels are higher than
perceived norms (i.e. greater underestimation in perception). The ±
values represent the margin of error (half of the 95% confidence
interval for each state’s mean).
Variation by demographics
We regressed pluralistic ignorance across
items in a linear mixed model weighted to be nationally
representative, with random intercept for participant and item, on the
full battery of demographics assessed (see Methods). As shown in
Supplementary Table 2,
we find a number of statistically significant factors. Consistent with
false consensus effects, we find participant’s political orientation
has a notable effect (with 22% underestimation for those who are very
liberal to 33% for those who are very conservative). Race also has a
notable impact, with 25% underestimation for white respondents to 35%
underestimation for black respondents, and other races falling in
between. Other demographic characteristics had smaller, but still
statistically significant effects. For instance, those living in urban
areas were about 29% off, while their suburban counterparts were 26%
off (and rural respondents falling in between).
Notably, there was no demographic group
for which the estimated range reached accurate levels—instead all
groups assessed were at least 20% off. Further, some demographics
which might have been anticipated to predict reduced misperceptions
did have statistically significant effects, but were small shifts in
absolute terms: Those who attended 12 years of schooling but never
obtained a GED or diploma were 28% off, while those with a doctorate
were still 27% off, just a single percentage point better.
Variation by local norms
In exploratory analyses, we assessed two
state level predictors for their relationship to pluralistic ignorance
levels across items with both known real and perceived levels: the
voting margin for Biden in the 2020 election (used as a state-level
proxy for prevalent political ideology), and the logged number of
climate or environmental protests per capita (see Methods). The
effects of these predictors were assessed in a multiple regression
mixed model with random intercepts for participant and item, and
controlling for the top five demographic variables shown to have an
effect and likely vary by state (personal political orientation, race,
employment status, age, and income). Consistent with an availability
heuristic, we find that both indicators of local norms influence norm
estimates: there is a significant effect for state political ideology
b = −0.02, t(39760) = 2.35, P = 0.019, such that
states with the highest margin for Biden had pluralistic ignorance
levels of 25.5%, while those with highest margin for Trump were 28.1%
off. We also find a significant effect for state-level environmental
protests b = −0.47, t(39980) = 2.32, P = 0.020,
such that states with the highest level of protest were estimated to
have pluralistic ignorance levels of 25.9%, while those with the
fewest protests were 28.3% off.
Variation by media consumption
In exploratory analyses, we assessed the
relationship between news media consumption and pluralistic ignorance
levels across items. Using a dummy-coded variable to compare consumers
of each news outlet (those who view it at least weekly) to those who
do not, we assess the effect of media consumption for each outlet in a
multiple regression mixed model with random intercepts for participant
and item. And as media consumption may vary based on demographics, we
control for the full battery of demographics assessed here, including
personal political orientation, education, age, race, and income. We
find that consumers of all news media outlets underestimate climate
concern and policy support by around 25–30% (see Fig. 5,
Panel A). Contrasting the differences between viewers and non-viewers
of each outlet (see Fig. 5,
Panel B), we see relatively lower levels for consumers of public
broadcasting (National Public Radio), and mainstream news outlets
including major national papers (e.g., New York Times), major cable
news outlets (e.g., CNN), national broadcast news networks (e.g.,
ABC). We see relatively higher levels for those who consume news from
major conservative outlets like Fox News and other conservative
outlets (e.g., Breitbart), as well as for other liberal outlets (e.g.,
The Nation).
A shows pluralistic ignorance levels
for consumers of each news media source. B Shows effects on
pluralistic ignorance levels comparing consumers to non-consumers of
each news media source (zero = baseline levels of misestimates).
Estimates in both panels are obtained from a mixed model contrasting
viewers and non-viewers of each news source and controlling for
participants’ demographics. Pluralistic ignorance levels of N = 6119
participants are calculated by taking the difference from real and
perceived levels of policy support and climate concern (greater values
indicate real levels are higher than perceived norms, i.e. greater
underestimation in perception), and controlling for the battery of
demographics assessed (see Methods). Error bars are 95% confidence
intervals of the means illustrated.
This pattern generally suggests that
media exposure to outlets with less favorable coverage of climate
change policy correspond with lower estimates of public support,
with the increase in pluralistic ignorance for those consuming
“other liberal outlets” as the exception. Notably, “other liberal
outlets” was also the least consumed outlet, with only 15.5%
regularly consuming it (95% CI = [14.6, 16.4]), while all other
outlets were consumed by 20–63% of Americans. One possibility is
that consumers of these liberal outlets recognize their news
source both liberal and niche, and therefore presume others do not
share their more liberal, pro-climate attitudes.
Discussion
We find that roughly 80–90% of Americans
underestimate the true level of concern for climate change as well
as support for transformative climate policies like a carbon tax,
100-percent renewable energy mandates, and a Green New Deal. Not
only are these misperceptions nearly universal in the country, but
the magnitude is large enough to fully invert the true reality of
public opinion: although polls show that a supermajority support
these climate policies (66–80%), the average American’s estimate
of public opinion suggests it is just a minority (37–3%, effect
sizes of the difference ranging from d = 0.91–1.48). In
other words, supporters of major climate policies outnumber
opponents 2 to 1, but Americans falsely perceive nearly the
opposite to be true. In fact, Americans’ estimates for all
national support for climate policies is roughly the same or even
lower than even just Republican levels of support.
This misperception is highly robust,
being present for all the climate policies assessed here, and true
across the country: Americans in every state and of all major
demographics are 20% or more off in their estimates of support for
all climate policies. In all cases, Americans failed to understand
that a strong majority of fellow Americans support climate policy,
instead, estimating it to be a minority. Given both the ubiquity
and magnitude of misperception, this represents a notable form of
pluralistic ignorance, perhaps best described as a false social
reality, defined here as a case where an inverted perception of
the attitudes of others is nearly held by all in a society.
We also find preliminary evidence for
possible sources of this misperception. Our results are partially
consistent with previous theory and research on false consensus
effects:29
those who are less likely to support these policies
(conservatives) are more likely to underestimate climate policy
support by a greater degree. Our results are also consistent with
previous theory and research on the conservative bias22,
where people may anchor on more conservative historic levels of
political attitudes, failing to update estimates to match current
public opinion. Further, consistent with availability heuristics30,
salient information from one’s local norms, such as the political
ideology of those in one’s state, and the number of climate
protests one might observe in their state, are also linked to
these misperceptions, such that more liberal states and states
with more climate protests have somewhat lower misperceptions.
Finally, the news media that one consumes may also play a role:
those who consume conservative outlets are more likely to have
more erroneous views.
Beyond these contributing factors,
there are additional psychological mechanisms that may help
explain the effects found here. For example, it has been shown
that many liberals experience “false uniqueness” whereby they
falsely assume that their own opinions are less common than they
really are36,
which could explain why even liberals underestimate levels of
support for climate mitigation policies by a large degree.
Broadly, there are many psychological factors that are plausible
contributors to the misperceptions documented here and warrant
future research. Additionally, media consumption may correspond
with other demographic features not controlled for here and the
data used in analyses here are correlational in nature. So while
recent research finds that media consumption plays a causal role
in shaping political beliefs37,
one should exercise caution in presuming media effect cause the
pattern of results observed here until experimental data can
confirm such effects.
These results have a number of
concerning implications. The extent of pluralistic ignorance in
this context presents at least two major hurdles for climate
action. First, it undermines people’s willingness to discuss the
issue8
and thus obstructs organizing around it. And second, erroneously
enlarged perceptions of the opposition’s numbers should increase
conformity pressures to oppose climate policy13,
diminishing motivation and political pressure to pursue these
essential climate goals. Further, a perceived popular consensus
around climate change may be key to reducing polarization around
climate change as it can help bring conservatives closer to the
majority of Americans on the issue38—while
in the absence of this perceived consensus as seen here,
polarization may thrive. If so, these misperceptions represent be
a self-fulfilling prophecy: one where underappreciated levels of
support for climate policy inhibit support for climate solutions
needed, and undermine nascent efforts at substantive change.
The results of our study clearly
establish pluralistic ignorance in the US climate policy context,
and granularly maps it out, while offering an initial
investigation into possible sources. Given the host of ways in
which pluralistic ignorance may undermine climate policy support
and action, these findings may help us understand the historic
absence of major national climate policy despite solid majorities
of the American public favoring strong action and setting goals
such as net zero by 205039.
These results also underscore the need for future research to
investigate and document the variety of possible contributing
factors of pluralistic ignorance, including those explored here,
particularly by utilizing experimental and causal evidence. Such
work may help in developing and accessing practically relevant
interventions. Norm misperceptions have been addressed by
interventions in a variety of domains, such as those aimed at
increasing perceptions of tax compliance40,
reducing perceptions of heavy drinking on college campuses18,
and reducing perceptions of that school bullying is approved of41.
Our work suggests the importance of developing a similar
intervention in the climate policy context to correct pluralistic
ignorance and help empower efforts to pass transformative climate
policies.
Methods
Participants
We used the Ipsos eNation Omnibus
nationally representative panel to survey US adults (N = 6,119)
between April and May in 2021. Ipsos calibrates respondent
characteristics to be representative of the U.S. population where
source of these population targets is U.S. Census 2019 American
Community Survey data, including targets for region, gender, age,
and household income. To recruit a greater number of participants
from less populated states, Ipsos implemented a cap on the number
of participants from larger states (ending recruitment from states
after they reached N = 250). This method allowed Ipsos to
continue using their representative panel, but kept recruitment
open for participants in smaller states. While the aim was not to
recruit equal numbers from every state, this approach did improve
recruitment of participants from small states that otherwise would
have very small numbers. Ipsos provided weighed values used in all
calculations of national levels to ensure representativeness.
These post-hoc weights were made to the population characteristics
on gender, age, race/ethnicity, region, and education. Ipsos
implemented the following data quality control checks: removal of
participants who took less than half of the median time, those who
streaked responses in survey responses, and those who did not
complete the survey. This survey provider was chosen for its
high-quality data collection and for being the same provider used
in polling actual levels of concern and support for identical
items by the YPCCC, which is one of the most comprehensive polling
efforts done on US climate opinion11.
Materials and procedure
For all norm estimates, participants
responded using a free response question. Participants were asked
to estimate the percent of Americans who were at least somewhat
concerned about climate change, as well as the percent of
Americans who supported each of the following climate policies: a
carbon tax, a 100-percent renewable energy mandate for
electricity, siting renewables on public lands, and a Green New
Deal (GND). Each policy was shown given the same brief description
as used in polling by the YPCCC (see Table 2).
However, our phrasing did differ for the item about worry: while
we asked about worry in “climate change”, YPCCC asked about worry
in “global warming”. Another nationally representative sample of
Americans42
polled at the same time (early 2021) asked “How concerned are you
about global climate change?”, and found similar (slightly
greater) levels of concern about (72% at least “somewhat
concerned”) compared to YPCCC’s data on “global warming” (66% at
least “somewhat worried”). Differences in concern about climate
change and global warming may lead to different precise levels of
pluralistic ignorance. Participants were then asked to estimate
concern and support for the same policies among those in their
state of residence.
In comparing perceived levels to real
levels for the items in Table 1,
we use a one sample t-test against a constant. Comparing against a
constant value reflects the confidence held in the overall body of
work YPCCC has collected, sampling tens of thousands of
observations from nationally representative polls regularly for
over a decade. However, one could choose to ignore the broader
body of work and use a two-sample t-test comparing the
perceived value to the specific poll selected for comparison.
Doing so does not meaningfully change the results (Supplementary
Table 1).
Next participants were asked for their
media consumption of the following outlets: “Mainstream cable news
(CNN, MSNBC)”, “New York Times, Huffington Post, or the Washington
Post”, “News from ABC, CBS, NBC, or similar local or national TV
broadcast news”, “NPR (radio or online)”, “Fox Cable News”, “Other
conservative news, shows or radio (Breitbart, Drudge Report,
Newsmax, Rush Limbaugh, The Blaze, OAN, etc.)”, and “Liberal news
outlets (Democracy Now, The Intercept, The Nation, Salon, Mother
Jones, Common Dreams, etc.)”. These were shown in a matrix with
the following frequency options: “Never”, “Every few weeks”, “Once
a week”, “Every few days”, “Almost every day or more”.
We used up-to-date polling data
available from YPCCC, including the polling results published in
2021 on worry about climate change43
and climate policy items12.
For the state levels of environmental
and climate protests, we utilized the protest event data collected
by the Crowd Counting Consortium44.
Of these records, we selected protests from the past 5 years
published in May 2021, and pertaining to climate, the environment,
and/or energy. This dataset includes protests events with as few
as 1 people in attendance, and many of these smaller events lack
clear confirmation. Therefore, we only included protests with at
least 100 attendees. This yielded 1046 protests of interest spread
across the US. We then calculated protests per capita using the
2019 US Census estimates for each states’ population and logged
the result to obtain a fairly normal distribution (skew = −0.28,
kurtosis = 1.2; vs. skew = 6, kurtosis = 58.1 when not logged).
Demographic variables provided by
IPSOS include political orientation, age, race, gender, education,
income, employment, marriage and housing status, number of
children, and whether participants live in an urban, rural or
suburban area. This research was approved by an Internal Review
Board at the home institution of the corresponding author and
informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants
provided their informed consent prior to completing the survey.
Data were analyzed in R (version 3.6.1)45.
All data analyzed are included in the
supplementary data files (Supplementary Data File 1
for participant data; Supplementary Data File 2
for a participant data codebook; Supplementary Data File 3
for state-level data used; and Source Data for all figure source
data). Source
data are provided with this paper.
References
Fischer, P. et al. The
bystander-effect: a meta-analytic review on bystander
intervention in dangerous and non-dangerous emergencies.
Psychol. Bull.137, 517–537 (2011).
Biel, A. & Thøgersen, J.
Activation of social norms in social dilemmas: a review of
the evidence and reflections on the implications for
environmental behaviour. J. Econ. Psychol.28,
93–112 (2007).
Bolsen, T., Leeper, T. J. &
Shapiro, M. A. Doing what others do: norms, science, and
collective action on global warming. Am. Politics Res.42, 65–89 (2014).
Howe, L. C., Carr, P. B. & Walton,
G. M. Normative appeals motivate people to contribute to
collective action problems more when they invite people to
work together toward a common goal. J. Personal. Soc.
Psychol.121, 215–238 (2021).
Maibach, E. et al. Oxford
Research Encyclopedia of Climate Science (Oxford
University Press, 2016)
Howe, P. D., Mildenberger, M.,
Marlon, J. R. & Leiserowitz, A. Geographic variation in
opinions on climate change at state and local scales in the
USA. Nat. Clim. Change5, 596–603 (2015).
Leiserowitz, A. et al. Politics
& Global Warming (Climate Change Communication, 2021).
Yeager, D., Krosnick, J., Visser,
P., Holbrook, A. Tahk, A. et al. Moderation of classic
social psychological effects by demographics in the U.S.
adult population: new opportunities for theoretical
advancement J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 117,
e84–e99 (2019).
Searle, J. R. The Construction
of Social Reality (Free Press, 1995).
Durkheim, E. Rules of
Sociological Method (Simon and Schuster, 1982).
Snyder, M. & Swann, W. B.
Behavioral confirmation in social interaction: from social
perception to social reality. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.14, 148–162 (1978).
Sargent, R. H. & Newman, L. S.
Pluralistic ignorance research in psychology: a scoping
review of topic and method variation and directions for
future research. Rev. Gen. Psychol.25,
163–184 (2021).
Prentice, D. A. & Miller, D. T.
Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: Some
consequences of misperceiving the social norm. J.
Personal. Soc. Psychol.64, 243–256 (1993).
Ballew, M. T. et al.,
“Americans Underestimate How Many Others in the U.S. Think
Global Warming is Happening” (Yale University and George
Mason University, 2019).
Leviston, Z., Walker, I. &
Morwinski, S. Your opinion on climate change might not be as
common as you think. Nat. Clim. Change3,
334–337 (2013).
Mildenberger, M. & Tingley, D.
Beliefs about climate beliefs: the importance of
second-order opinions for climate politics. Br. J.
Political Sci.49, 1279–1307 (2019).
Ballew, M. T. et al. Beliefs about
others’ global warming beliefs: the role of party
affiliation and opinion deviance. J. Environ. Psychol.70, 101466 (2020).
Sokoloski, R., Markowitz, E. M. &
Bidwell, D. Public estimates of support for offshore wind
energy: False consensus, pluralistic ignorance, and partisan
effects. Energy Policy112, 45–55 (2018).
Ross, L., Greene, D. & House, P.
The “false consensus effect”: an egocentric bias in social
perception and attribution processes. J. Exp. Soc.
Psychol.13, 279–301 (1977).
Larson, E. et al. Net-Zero
America by 2050: Potential Pathways, Deployments and Impacts
(Princeton University, 2021).
Bergquist, P., Mildenberger, M. &
Stokes, L. C. Combining climate, economic, and social policy
builds public support for climate action in the US.
Environ. Res. Lett.15, 054019 (2020).
Broockman, D. & Kalla, J. The
Manifold Effects of Partisan Media on Viewers’ Beliefs and
Attitudes: A Field Experiment with Fox News Viewers.
https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/jrw26 (2022).
Sabherwal, A., Pearson, A. R. &
Sparkman, G. Anger consensus messaging can enhance
expectations for collective action and support for climate
mitigation. J. Environ. Psycholo.76,
101640 (2021).
Communications, U. Florida
Residents Continue to Worry About Climate Change and Various
Effects, Saint Leo University Survey Shows. Polling
Institute at Saint Leo University
https://polls.saintleo.edu/6373-2/ (2022).
A. Leiserowitz, et al. Climate
Change in the American Mind (Climate Change
Communication, 2021).
Crowd Counting Consortium.
Crowd Counting Consortium Data (GitHub, 2021).
R Core Team. R. R: A Language
and Environment for Statistical Computing (Springer
Nature, 2022).
We thank Climate Central for their
support and feedback on this research. The preparation of this
manuscript was supported by National Science Foundation grant,
SES-DRMS 2018063 to E.U.W. and G.S.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Department of Psychology and
Neuroscience, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, MA, 02467, USA
Gregg Sparkman
Media School, Indiana University
Bloomington, Bloomington, IN, 47405, USA
Nathan Geiger
Andlinger Center for Energy and
Environment, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, 08540, USA
Elke U. Weber
Contributions
G.S., N.G. and E.U.W. designed the
research, G.S. and N.G. collected the data, G.S. and N.G. analyzed
the data, and G.S., N.G., and E.U.W. wrote the paper.
Nature Communications thanks
Magnus Bergquist, Peter Howe, and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s)
for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer
reviewer reports are available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature
remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published
maps and institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is
licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and
reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give
appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if
changes were made. The images or other third party material in
this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons
license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative
Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by
statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need
to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a
copy of this license, visit
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.